Monday, May 23, 2016

Bayesian reasoning implicated in some mental disorders

From within the dark confines of the skull, the brain builds its own version of reality. By weaving together expectations and information gleaned from the senses, the brain creates a story about the outside world. For most of us, the brain is a skilled storyteller, but to spin a sensible yarn, it has to fill in some details itself.
“The brain is a guessing machine, trying at each moment of time to guess what is out there,” says computational neuroscientist Peggy Seriès.
Guesses just slightly off — like mistaking a smile for a smirk — rarely cause harm. But guessing gone seriously awry may play a part in mental illnesses such as schizophrenia, autism and even anxiety disorders, Seriès and other neuroscientists suspect. They say that a mathematical expression known as Bayes’ theorem — which quantifies how prior expectations can be combined with current evidence — may provide novel insights into pernicious mental problems that have so far defied explanation.

Expectations

People have assumptions about the world, which are either inborn or learned early in life. For example:
  • Light comes from above.
  • Noses stick out.
  • Objects move slowly.
  • Background images are uniformly colored.
  • Other people’s gazes are directed at us.
Bayes’ theorem “offers a new vocabulary, new tools and a new way to look at things,” says Seriès, of the University of Edinburgh.
Experiments guided by Bayesian math reveal that the guessing process differs in people with some disorders. People with schizophrenia, for instance, can have trouble tying together their expectations with what their senses detect. And people with autism and high anxiety don’t flexibly update their expectations about the world, some lab experiments suggest. That missed step can muddy their decision-making abilities.
Given the complexity of mental disorders such as schizophrenia and autism, it is no surprise that many theories of how the brain works have fallen short, says psychiatrist and neuroscientist Rick Adams of University College London. Current explanations for the disorders are often vague and untestable. Against that frustrating backdrop, Adams sees great promise in a strong mathematical theory, one that can be used to make predictions and actually test them.
“It’s really a step up from the old-style cognitive psychology approach, where you had flowcharts with boxes and labels on them with things like ‘attention’ or ‘reading,’ but nobody having any idea about what was going on in [any] box,” Adams says.
Applying math to mental disorders “is a very young field,” he adds, pointing to Computational Psychiatry, which plans to publish its first issue this summer. “You know a field is young when it gets its first journal.”

A mind for math

Bayesian reasoning may be new to the mental illness scene, but the math itself has been around for centuries. First described by the Rev. Thomas Bayes in the 18th century, this computational approach truly embraces history: Evidence based on previous experience, known as a “prior,” is essential to arriving at a good answer, Bayes argued. He may have been surprised to see his math meticulously applied to people with mental illness, but the logic holds. To make a solid guess about what’s happening in the world, the brain must not rely just on current input from occasionally unreliable senses. The brain must also use its knowledge about what has happened before. Merging these two streams of information correctly is at the heart of perceiving the world as accurately as possible.
Bayes figured out a way to put numbers to this process. By combining probabilities that come from prior evidence and current observations, Bayes’ formula can be used to calculate an overall estimate of the likelihood that a given suspicion is true. A properly functioning brain seems to do this calculation intuitively, behaving in many cases like a skilled Bayesian statistician, some studies show (SN: 10/8/11, p. 18).
Story continues after graphic

Where there's smoke

The example below shows how Bayesian reasoning is applied to a question of the probability of a dangerous fire.
This reckoning requires the brain to give the right amount of weight to prior expectations and current information. Depending on the circumstances, those weights change. When the senses falter, for instance, the brain should lean more heavily on prior expectations. Say the mail carrier comes each day at 4 p.m. On a stormy afternoon when visual cues are bad, we rely less on sight and more on prior knowledge to guess that the late-afternoon noise on the front porch is probably the mail carrier delivering letters. In certain mental illnesses, this flexible balancing act may falter.
People with schizophrenia often suffer from hallucinations and delusions, debilitating symptoms that arise when lines between reality and imagination blur. That confusion can lead to hearing voices that aren’t there and believing things that can’t possibly be true. These departures from reality could arise from differences in how people integrate new evidence with previous beliefs.
There’s evidence for such distorted calculations. People with schizophrenia don’t fall for certain visual illusions that trick most people, for instance. When shown a picture of the inside of a hollowed-out face mask, most people’s brains mistakenly convert the image to a face that pops outward off the page. People with schizophrenia, however, are more likely to see the face as it actually is — a concave mask. In that instance, people with schizophrenia give more weight to information that’s coming from their eyes than to their expectation that noses protrude from the rest of the face. To complicate matters, the opposite can be true, too, says neuropsychologist Chris Frith of the Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging at University College London. “In this case, their prior is too weak, but in other cases, their prior is too strong,” he says.
In a recent study, healthy people and those who recently began experiencing psychosis, a symptom of schizophrenia, were shown confusing shadowy black-and-white images. Participants then saw color versions of the images that were easier to interpret. When shown the black-and-white images again, people with early psychosis were better at identifying the images, suggesting that they used their prior knowledge — the color pictures — to truly “see” the images. For people without psychosis, the color images weren’t as much help. That difference suggests that the way people with schizophrenia balance past knowledge and present observations is distinct from the behavior of people without the disorder. Sometimes the balance tips too far — in either direction.
In a talk at the annual Computational and Systems Neuroscience meeting in February in Salt Lake City, Seriès described the results of a different visual test: A small group of people with schizophrenia had to describe which way a series of dots were moving on a screen. The dots moved in some directions more frequently than others — a statistical feature that let the scientists see how well people could learn to predict the dots’ directions. The 11 people with schizophrenia seemed just as good at learning which way the dots were likely to move as the 10 people without, Seriès said. In this situation, people with schizophrenia seemed able to learn priors just fine.
But when another trick was added, a split between the two groups emerged. Sometimes, the dots were almost impossible to see, and sometimes, there were no dots at all. People with schizophrenia were less likely to claim that they saw dots when the screen was blank. Perhaps they didn’t hallucinate dots because of the medication they were on, Seriès says. In fact, very early results from unmedicated people with schizophrenia suggest that they actually see dots that aren’t there more than healthy volunteers.
Preliminary results so far on schizophrenia are sparse and occasionally conflicting, Seriès admits. “It’s the beginning,” she says. “We don’t understand much.”
The research is so early that no straightforward story exists yet. But that’s not unexpected. “If 100 years of schizophrenia research have taught us anything, it’s that there’s not going to be a nice, simple explanation,” Adams says. But using math to describe how people perceive the world may lead to new hunches about how that process goes wrong in mental illnesses, he argues.
“You can instill expectations in subjects in many different ways, and you can control what evidence they see,” Adams says. Bayesian theory “tells you what they should conclude from those prior beliefs and that evidence.” If their conclusions diverge from predictions, scientists can take the next step. Brain scans, for instance, may reveal how the wrong answers arise. With a clear description of these differences, he says, “we might be able to measure people’s cognition in a new way, and diagnose their disorders in a new way.”

Now vs. then

The way the brain combines incoming sensory information with existing knowledge may also be different in autism, some researchers argue. In some cases, people with autism might put excess weight on what their senses take in about the world and rely less on their expectations. Old observations fit with this idea. In the 1960s, psychologists had discovered that children with autism were just as good at remembering nonsense sentences (“By is go tree stroke lets”) as meaningful ones (“The fish swims in the pond”). Children without autism struggled to remember the non sequiturs. But the children with autism weren’t thrown by the random string of words, suggesting that their expectations of sentence meaning weren’t as strong as their ability to home in on each word in the series.
Another study supports the notion that sensory information takes priority in people with autism. People with and without autism were asked to judge whether a sight and a sound happened at the same time. They saw a white ring on a screen, and a tone played before, after or at the same time. Adults without autism were influenced by previous trials in which the ring and tone were slightly off. But adults with autism were not swayed by earlier trials, researchers reported in February in Scientific Reports.
This literal perception might get in the way of speech perception, Marco Turi of the University of Pisa in Italy and colleagues suggest. Comprehending speech requires a listener to mentally stitch together sights and sounds that may not arrive at the eyes and ears at the same time. Losing that flexibility could make speech harder to understand.
A different study found that children with autism perceive moving dots more clearly than children without autism (SN Online: 5/5/15). The brains of people with autism seem to prioritize incoming sensory information over expectations about how things ought to work. Elizabeth Pellicano of University College London and David Burr of the University of Western Australia in Perth described the concept in 2012 in an opinion paper in Trends in Cognitive Sciences. Intensely attuned to information streaming in from the senses, people with autism experience the world as “too real,” Pellicano and Perth wrote.
New data, however, caution against a too-simple explanation. In an experiment presented in New York City in April at the annual meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society, 20 adults with and without autism had to quickly hit a certain key on a keyboard when they saw its associated target on a screen. Their job was made easier because the targets came in a certain sequence. All of the participants improved as they learned which keys to expect. But when the sequence changed to a new one, people with autism faltered. This result suggests that they learned prior expectations just fine, but had trouble updating them as conditions changed, said cognitive neuroscientist Owen Parsons of the University of Cambridge.
Story continues after graphic

Memory test

In a 1967 study, children with autism were just as good at remembering nonsense strings of words as they were at remembering sentences. The results suggest that they had weaker expectations about meaningful sentences.
Distorted calculations — and the altered versions of the world they create — may also play a role in depression and anxiety, some researchers think. While suffering from depression, people may hold on to distorted priors — believing that good things are out of reach, for instance. And people with high anxiety can have trouble making good choices in a volatile environment, neuroscientist Sonia Bishop of the University of California, Berkeley and colleagues reported in 2015 in Nature Neuroscience.
In their experiment, people had to choose a shape, which sometimes  came with a shock. People with low anxiety quickly learned to avoid the shock, even when the relationship between shape and shock changed. But people with high anxiety performed worse when those relationships changed, the researchers found. “High-anxious individuals didn’t seem able to adjust their learning to handle how volatile or how stable the environment was,” Bishop says.

Stress shutdown

People with higher levels of anxiety (light blue) learned less from shocks (measured by changes in pupil size) when the environment changed than people with low anxiety (dark blue).
Source: Michael Browning et al/Nature Neuroscience 2015.
Scientists can’t yet say what causes this difficulty adjusting to a new environment in anxious people and in people with autism. It could be that once some rule is learned (a sequence of computer keys, or the link between a shape and a shock), these two groups struggle to update that prior with newer information.
This rigidity might actually contribute to anxiety in the first place, Bishop speculates. “When something unexpected happens that is bad, you wouldn’t know how to respond,” and that floundering “is likely to be a huge source of anxiety and stress.”

Recalculating

“There’s been a lot of frustration with a failure to make progress” on psychiatric disorders, Bishop says. Fitting mathematical theories to the brain may be a way to move forward. Researchers “are very excited about computational psychiatry in general,” she says.
Computational psychiatrist Quentin Huys of the University of Zurich is one of those people. Math can help clarify mental illnesses in a way that existing approaches can’t, he says. In the March issue of Nature Neuroscience, Huys and colleagues argued that math can demystify psychiatric disorders, and that thinking of the brain as a Bayesian number cruncher might lead to a more rigorous understanding of mental illness. Huys says that a computational approach is essential. “We can’t get away without it.” If people with high anxiety perform differently on a perceptual test, then that test could be used to both diagnose people and monitor how well a treatment works, for instance.
Scientists hope that a deeper description of mental illnesses may lead to clearer ways to identify a disorder, chart how well treatments work and even improve therapies. Bishop raises the possibility of developing apps to help people with high anxiety evaluate situations  — outsourcing the decision making for people who have trouble. Frith points out that cognitive behavioral therapy could help depressed people recalculate their experiences by putting less weight on negative experiences and perhaps breaking out of cycles of despondence.
Beyond these potential interventions, simply explaining to people how their brains are working might ease distress, Adams says. “If you can give people an explanation that makes sense of some of the experiences they’ve had, that can be a profoundly helpful thing,” he says. “It destigmatizes the experience.”

This article appears in the May 28, 2016, Science News with the headline, "Misguided math: Faulty Bayesian reasoning may explain some mental disorders."

Friday, May 20, 2016

Military Apologizes After Drone Strike Intended For Yemeni ISIS Base Accidentally Hits West Palm Beach Wedding







A who's who at Brooklyn's coolest innovation hub


On the banks of the East River, across 35 acres of land and comprised of some 16 buildings is Industry City, the largest privately owned industrial complex in New York. Inside this Brooklyn innovation ecosystem is a diverse concentration of more than 400 companies (and growing!) that tinker, build, create and design. Get a snapshot of the movers and shakers at this Sunset Park outpost by reading about some of the innovators who call Industry City home.

Aerobo

Aerobo is the first FAA-approved drone operator in New York and the only company in the country operating drones on such a large scale. More specifically, though: The company makes flying robots.
Processed with VSCOcam with s2 preset
On a typical Monday morning, Aerobo's chief pilot can be found cleaning dust from the controllers in the lab next door to its office inside Industry City. A sort of machine workshop meets drone incubator, the lab is where engineers bench test each motor they build, measuring things like thrust, output, temperature, draw and amperage to make sure everything is flight-ready.
Processed with VSCOcam with s2 preset
This is also where they create prototypes using a 3D printer and cut parts from a sheet of carbon fiber. It can get a little loud, but that's precisely why the headquarters are there.
"Having thick concrete walls is important," says company CMO Jon Ollwerther, above the clamor of machinery on the other side of the wall. "Having an office in Midtown Manhattan was not an option."
Aerobo CEO and co-founder Brian Streem counts Industry City's collegial, collaborative vibe as a big part of why he loves having their office here. In fact, when Aerobo first moved into the space, they borrowed WiFi from the people across the hall while getting their own connection set up. It's the modern equivalent of borrowing a cup of sugar.

Material Wrld

As co-founders of the luxury fashion trade-in company Material Wrld, Jie Zheng and Rie Yano have a mutual love for how the digital age can support their love for fashion. The friends, who met at Harvard Business School, both have a background in fashion e-commerce and were inspired to start their company when they realized the tired consignment store model of re-sale deserved an upgrade.
materialwrld
Material Wrld accepts trade-ins through the mail, which are processed by a team of buyers, purchased directly from customers and then resold via their sister site, The World, and eBay. Price offers are based on a combination of data, including market value of the brand, category, style, condition and seasonality. Once the customer accepts the offer, they can use that money to make new purchases using the Material Wrld Fashion Trade-In Card or through gift cards.
By locating operations at Industry City, Material Wrld has been able to keep inventory and operations all under one roof. It's a smart and economical move for a startup looking to streamline the process, learn from challenges that arise on site and become more efficient.
materialwrld2
It doesn't hurt that on-campus activities create a fun and collaborative atmosphere among the tenants either. Yano and Zheng say the open vibe of the food court, plus sponsored activities like summer happy hours, rooftop films and daytime parties by Mister Sunday, means there are multiple opportunities for getting to know the neighbors.
"There's a really great atmosphere here where you can create, meet and collaborate with other companies," Yano says.

Danielle Trofe Design

Though Danielle Trofe's studio may be small — all told, it's just about 250 square feet — her ideas are big. Trofe creates sustainable solutions for everyday needs, using eco-friendly materials and techniques in a range of creative ways. Her most recent endeavor is MushLume, a line of lamps grown from mushroom mycelium.
Trofe's goal is to challenge how objects are made, how the materials are used and where they wind up at the end of their lives. Each of the organic lampshades in her collection is made from a rapidly renewable resource — a combination of corn stalk, seed husk and hemp held together with liquid mushroom mycelium that grows and solidifies — and is completely biodegradable. (Watch a video about her process here.)
At the heart of everything she does, from the mushroom lamps to a family of hydroponic vertical planters, is a curiosity for exploring how science, technology and design can come together in new and exciting ways.
danielle-trofe
Danielle Trofe designed the vertical planters that stand in front of the windows in her studio.
The best part about setting up her operation at Industry City though is the massive array of windows that line an entire wall of her studio, plus the interior windows that allow her and fellow tenants to get a peek into what else is going on in the buildings.
"I like this idea of, 'Let's see how things are actually made.' Having that open-door, open-window policy is kind of nice," Trofe says.

Coinspace

Walk into the Coinspace headquarters and it’s easy to see that founder, Solomon Lederer, is a fan of how traditional models can be re-invented in new ways. Along one wall is a gallery of framed currency, from a One Hundred Trillion Zimbabwean Dollar, Euros and a Saudi Arabian Riyal note to $100,000 worth of Monopoly money. Also on the wall is a Bitcoin paper wallet, which is central to the operation.
CoinspaceImage3
Coinspace is an incubator of sorts, a co-working space for start-ups focused on cryptocurrencies like Bitcoin and Ethereum, plus transactional databases using blockchain technology and beyond. Any day of the week, you may find members using the bespoke wood tables and mixed assortment of mid-century modern chairs to conduct business and change the way we think about money.
MoneyWall
A gallery of framed currency includes notes in circulation, plus those both fictional and obsolete.
Lederer's goal is to practice a unique brand of digital socialism in the space, combining a shared economy with a decentralized autonomous organization that’s largely democratic. In practice, that means when something needs to be done — like hanging a mirror — one member can pay another through Coinspace coins to get it done. The vision is to eventually create a situation where the space can operate on its own, paying for transactional services like Internet or rent through cryptocurrency — no humans required.
Lederer says he loves Industry City because of its convenience and amenities, like the popular food halls, and the low rents in comparison to Manhattan. After all, money talks.
7-IC Food Hall
The food hall at Industry City features a range of options for a quick bite, plus a full-service restaurant.
Image: Industry City

HERO

It's an exciting time for HERO, manufacturers of a smart pill management device that’s about to go into production. At the company's 8,000 square foot space in Industry City, boxes of parts and shipping materials sit by a row of tables that will soon become the assembly line. Further back is a machine shop of sorts, where engineers design, test and build the electronic brains of the operation. Deeper still is the actual operations office, where a row of photos hang on the wall illustrating their customer segments.
HERO uses a patented pick and drop mechanism to deliver daily pills and vitamins, with a user-friendly interface and corresponding app that allows customers to set reminders and check in on loved ones to ensure medication is taken properly. The device also includes optional password protection to keep pills safe and secure.
Founder Kal Vepuri says that moving into the enormous space at Industry City a year ago has allowed the company to rapidly grow as they ramp up production, attracting talent nearby from Brooklyn and the rest of New York.
"[Industry City] allows us to have sufficient space for engineering, testing and doing research and development around a physical device, plus storing materials and assembling those devices," Vepuri says.
He also loves being in such an inspired community, working just down the hall from other entrepreneurs, makers and progressive thinkers.
"It was important to us to have a community doing innovative and progressive projects," he says.